Using ethnographic approach and cultural models framework in research on recreational drug use
stmm. 2020 (3): 178-193
DOI https://doi.org/10.15407/sociology2020.03.178
IEVGENIIA-GALYNA LUKASH,
2-nd year PhD-student in Sociology, National University of “Kyiv-Mohyla Academy” (2, Hryhorii Skovoroda St., Kyiv 04655)
ie.lukash@ukma.edu.ua
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-7296-3178
KATERYNA MALTSEVA,
Candidate of Sciences in Philosophy (2003), PhD in Anthropology (2010), Associate Professor at the Sociology Department of the Faculty of Social Sciences and Social Technologies, National University of “Kyiv-Mohyla Academy” (2, Hryhorii Skovoroda St., Kyiv 04655)
maltsevaKS@ukma.edu.ua
https://orcid.org/0000-0001-6540-8734
Substance abuse is one of the pressing issues that loom large in socio-epidemiological and health research in many countries nowadays. The empirical research on drug use is abundant, as are the perspectives on studying the risks associated with different aspects of drug use. In our article we give an overview of both prevalent and novel approaches to understanding the antecedents of drug use, focusing on the methodological means to create the ethnographically informed accounts of the reasons why individuals may start using drugs and how they themselves see this practice and their lives. The goal of the present publication is to outline the methodological benefits of the strategic use of the principles of ethnographic approach to various forms of data collection, and specifically looking into the intellectual framework of cultural models in applied research on recreational drug use. Ethnographic research on cultural models offers ample opportunities for methodological innovation, involving combined use of different techniques and integration of multimodal research options, and is particularly valuable for applied contexts due to the richness of the produced narrative. Using the methodological means supplied by the fieldwork-oriented research in drug use studies would offer new insights for scholars and policy makers. We present the methodological argument regarding the strategic use of the principles of ethnographic approach to increase the informativeness, accuracy and validity of the results in applied research on recreational drug use. Besides the methodological innovations the fieldwork-oriented research offers, using the methodological means supplied by the ethnographic research on cultural models would enable the social researchers to address the problem of drug use more efficiently.
Keywords: ethnographic methods, cultural models theory, recreational drug use, research design
Publication in: eng
References
Ahmed, S. (2011). Toward an evolutionary basis for resilience to drug addiction. Behavioral аnd Brain Sciences, 34 (6), 310-311. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x11000677
Banaschewski, T., Blomeyer, D., Buchmann, A., Poustka, L., Rothenberger, A., Laucht, M. (2011). Drugs as instruments from a developmental child and adolescent psychiatric perspective. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34 (6), 312-313. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x11000690
Becker, H. (1966). Outsiders: Studies in the Sociology of Deviance. London: Free Press.
Becker, H. (1967). History, Culture and Subjective Experience: An Exploration of the Social Bases of Drug-Induced Experiences. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 8 (3), 163. https://doi.org/10.2307/2948371
Bennardo, G., de Munck, V. (2013). Cultural models: Genesis, methods, and experiences. S.l.: Oxford University Press.
Blount, B. (2011). A history of cognitive anthropology. In: D.B. Kronenfeld, G. Bennardo, V.C. de Munck, M. Fischer (Eds.), A Companion to Cognitive Anthropology (pp. 11–29). Oxford: Blackwell. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781444394931
Bourgois, P. (2018). Decolonising drug studies in an era of predatory accumulation. Third World Quarterly, 39 (2), 385-398. https://doi.org/10.1080/01436597.2017.1411187
Caulkin, D.D. (2004). Identifying culture as a threshold of shared knowledge: A Consensus analysis method. International Journal of Cross Cultural Management, 4 (3), 317–333. https://doi.org/10.1177/1470595804047813
Chiu et al. (2010). Intersubjective culture: The role of intersubjective perceptions in cross-cultural research. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5 (4), 482–493. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691610375562
Commonly Abused Drugs (2020). Retrieved from: https://www.drugabuse.gov/sites/default/files/cadc...
D’Andrade, R. (1995). The Development of Cognitive Anthropology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
D’Andrade, R. (2002). Violence without honor in the American South. In: T. Aase (Ed.), Tournaments of Power: Honor and Revenge in the Contemporary World (pp. 61–75). Burlington: Ashgate. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315235950
D’Andrade, R. (2008). Study of Personal and Cultural Values: American, Japanese and Vietnamese. New York, NY: Palgrave Macmillan.
D’Andrade, R., Strauss, C. (1992). Human Motives and Cultural Models. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Dasgupta, N., Beletsky, L., Ciccarone, D. (2018). Opioid crisis: No easy fix to its social and economic determinants. American Journal of Public Health, 108 (2), 182–186. https://doi.org/10.2105/ajph.2017.304187
de Munck, V., Bennardo, G. (2019). Disciplining сulture: A sociocognitive approach. Current Anthropology, 60 (2), 174–193. https://doi.org/10.1086/702470
DiMaggio, P. (1997). Culture and cognition. Annual Review of Sociology, 23, (1), 263–87.
European Drug Report (2019). Retrieved from: http://www.emcdda.europa.eu/system/files/publicati...
European Monitoring Centre for Drugs and Drug Addiction (EMCDDA) (2017). Health and Social Responses to Drug Problems: A European Guide. S.l.
Foxall, G., Sigurdsson, V. (2011). Drug use as consumer behavior. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34(6), 313–314. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x11000707
Friedman, S., Mateu-Gelabert, P., Sandoval, M., Hagan, H., Jarlais, D. (2008). Positive deviance control-case life history: a method to develop grounded hypotheses about successful long-term avoidance of infection. BMC Public Health, 8(1). https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-8-94
Gilbert, M. (1987). Modeling collective belief. Synthese, 73, 185–204
Gilbert, M. (2015). Joint commitment. How we make a social world. New York: Oxford University Press.
Global Drug Survey (2016). Key findings from the Global Drug Survey 2016. https://doi.org/10.18356/fa014eb4-en
Goodenough, W. (1971). Culture, Language and Society. S.l.: Addison-Wesley.
Goudie, A., Gullo, M., Rose, A., Christiansen, P., Cole, J., Field, M., Sumnall, H. (2011). Nonaddictive instrumental drug use: Theoretical strengths and weaknesses. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34 (6), 314–315. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x11000719
Griffiths, M. (2011). Non-addictive psychoactive drug use: Implications for behavioral addiction. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34 (6), 315–316. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x11000720
Lareau, A. (2015). Cultural knowledge and social inequality. American Sociological Review, 80(1), 1–27.
Lee, C., Neighbors, C., Woods, B. (2007). Marijuana motives: Youngadults' reasons for using marijuana. Addictive Behaviors, 32(7), 1384–1394. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2006.09.010
Maltseva, K. (2016). Using correspondence analysis of scales as part of mixed methods design to access cultural models in ethnographic fieldwork: Prosocial cooperation in Sweden. Journal of Mixed Methods Research, 10 (1), 82–111. https://doi.org/10.1177/1558689814525262
Maltseva, K. (forthcoming). Bridging sociology with anthropology and cognitive science perspectives to assess shared cultural knowledge. _Sociology: Theory, Methods, Marketing._https://doi.org/10.15407/sociology2020.01.108
Maltseva, K., D’Andrade, R. (2011). Multi-item scales and cognitive ethnography. In: D.B. Kronenfeld, G. Bennardo, V.C. de Munck, M. Fischer (Eds.), A Companion to Cognitive Anthropology (pp. 153–170). Oxford: Blackwell. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781444394931.ch9
Miller, G. (2011). Optimal drug use and rational drug policy. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34 (6), 318–319. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x11000756
Moore, K., Miles, S. (2004). Young people, dance and the sub-cultural consumption of drugs. Addiction Research & Theory, 12 (6), 507–523. https://doi.org/10.1080/16066350412331323083
Müller, C., Schumann, G. (2011). Drugs as instruments: A new framework for non-addictive psychoactive drug use. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34 (6), 293–310. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x11000057
Nicholson, T., Duncan, D., White, J. (2002). Is recreational drug use normal? Journal of Substance Use, 7(3), 116–123. https://doi.org/10.1080/14659890209169340
Nutt, D., King, L., Saulsbury, W., Blakemore, C. (2007). Development of a rational scale to assess the harm of drugs of potential misuse. The Lancet, 369 (9566), 1047–1053. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0140-6736(07)60464-4
Pickard, H. (2011). The instrumental rationality of addiction. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34(6), 320-321. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x1100077x
Polavieja, J.G. (2015). Capturing culture: A new method to estimate exogenous cultural effects using migrant populations. American Sociological Review, 80(1), 166–191. https://doi.org/10.1177/0003122414562600
Prayag, G., Mura, P., Hall, C., Fontaine, J. (2016). Spirituality, drugs, and tourism: tourists’ and shamans’ experiences of ayahuasca in Iquitos, Peru. Tourism Recreation Research, 41(3), 314–325. https://doi.org/10.1080/02508281.2016.1192237
Quinn, N. (1996). Culture and contradiction: The case of Americans reasoning about marriage. Ethos, 24, 391–425. https://doi.org/10.1525/eth.1996.24.3.02a00010
Quinn, N. (2005). Finding Culture in Talk: A Collection of Methods. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.
Quinn, N. (2011). The history of the cultural models school reconsidered. In: D. B. Kronenfeld, G.Bennardo, V. C. de Munck, M. Fischer (Eds.), A Companion to Cognitive Anthropology (pp. 30–46). Oxford: Blackwell. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781444394931
Quinn, N. (2018). An anthropologist’s view of American marriage: Limitations of the tool kit theory of culture. In: N. Quinn (Ed.), Advances in Culture Theory from Psychological Anthropology (pp. 139–184). New York: Palgrave Macmillan. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-93674-1_6
Quinn, N., Holland, D. (1987). Culture and cognition. In: D. Holland, N. Quinn (Eds.), Cultural Models in Language and Thought (pp. 3–42). London: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/cbo9780511607660.002
Ravn, S., Duff, C. (2015). Putting the party down on paper: A novel method for mapping youth drug use in private settings. Health & Place, 31, 124–132. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.healthplace.2014.11.010
Ross, N. (2004). Culture and cognition: Implications for theory and method. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Seligmann, L., Estes, B. (2019). Innovations in ethnographic methods. American Behavioral Scientist, 64 (2), 176–197. https://doi.org/10.1177/0002764219859640
Shimizu, H. (2011). Cognitive approaches and education: foundational models of self and cultural models of teaching and learning in Japan and the United States. In: D.B. Kronenfeld, G. Bennardo, V.C. de Munck, M. Fischer (Eds.), A Companion to Cognitive Anthropology (pp. 430–449). Oxford: Blackwell. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781444394931.ch23
Romney, A.K., Weller, S. C., Batchelder, W.H. (1986). Culture as consensus: A theory of cultural and informant accuracy. American Anthropologist, 88, 313–338. https://doi.org/10.1525/aa.1986.88.2.02a00020
Singer, M., Page, J. (2016). The Social Value of Drug Addicts. London: Routledge.
Soussan, C., Andersson, M., Kjellgren, A. (2018). The diverse reasons for using Novel Psychoactive Substances - A qualitative study of the users' own perspectives. International Journal of Drug Policy, 52, 71–78. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugpo.2017.11.003
Strauss, C. (2000). The culture concept and the individualism/collectivism debate: Dominant and alternative attributions for class in the United States. In: L. Nucci, G. Saxe, E. Turiel (Eds.), Culture, Thought, and Development (pp. 85–114). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Suizzo, A.M. (2002). French parents’ cultural models and childrearing beliefs. _International Journal of Behavioral Development,_26, 297–307. https://doi.org/10.1080/01650250143000175
Swendsen, J., Le Moal, M. (2011). Flaws of drug instrumentalization. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34 (6), 323–324. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x11000732
Taylor, S.P., Hulsizer, M.R. (1998). Psychoactive drugs and human aggression. In: R.G. Geen, E. Donnerstein (Eds.), Human aggression: Theories, research, and implications for social policy (pp. 139–165). New York: Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/b978-012278805-5/50007-9
United Nations Office on Drugs and Crime (UNODC) (2018). World Drug Report 2018. United Nations publication, E.18.XI.9_._https://doi.org/10.18356/d29e3f27-en
Vaisey, S. (2009). Motivation and justification: A dual-process model of culture in action. American Journal of Sociology, 114, 1675–1715. https://doi.org/10.1086/597179
Van Gulick, R. (2011). Drugs, mental instruments, and self-control. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34 (6), 325–326. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x1100080x
Volkow, N., Collins, F. (2017). The role of science in addressing the opioid crisis. New England Journal of Medicine, 377 (4), 391–394. https://doi.org/10.1056/nejmsr1706626
Weller, S.C. (2007). Frequently asked questions about consensus analysis. _Field Methods,_19, 339–368. https://doi.org/10.1177/1525822x07303502
Weller, S.C., Romney, A.K. (1990). Metric Scaling. Correspondence Analysis. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Wu, K. (2011). Governing drug use through neurobiological subject construction: The sad loss of the sociocultural. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34 (6), 327–328. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x11000835
Zerubavel, E. (1999). Social Mindscapes. An Invitation to Cognitive Sociology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Received 12.04.2020
Using ethnographic approach and cultural models framework in research on recreational drug use
stmm. 2020 (3): 178-193
DOI https://doi.org/10.15407/sociology2020.03.178
IEVGENIIA-GALYNA LUKASH,
2-nd year PhD-student in Sociology, National University of “Kyiv-Mohyla Academy” (2, Hryhorii Skovoroda St., Kyiv 04655)
ie.lukash@ukma.edu.ua
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-7296-3178
KATERYNA MALTSEVA,
Candidate of Sciences in Philosophy (2003), PhD in Anthropology (2010), Associate Professor at the Sociology Department of the Faculty of Social Sciences and Social Technologies, National University of “Kyiv-Mohyla Academy” (2, Hryhorii Skovoroda St., Kyiv 04655)
maltsevaKS@ukma.edu.ua
https://orcid.org/0000-0001-6540-8734
Substance abuse is one of the pressing issues that loom large in socio-epidemiological and health research in many countries nowadays. The empirical research on drug use is abundant, as are the perspectives on studying the risks associated with different aspects of drug use. In our article we give an overview of both prevalent and novel approaches to understanding the antecedents of drug use, focusing on the methodological means to create the ethnographically informed accounts of the reasons why individuals may start using drugs and how they themselves see this practice and their lives. The goal of the present publication is to outline the methodological benefits of the strategic use of the principles of ethnographic approach to various forms of data collection, and specifically looking into the intellectual framework of cultural models in applied research on recreational drug use. Ethnographic research on cultural models offers ample opportunities for methodological innovation, involving combined use of different techniques and integration of multimodal research options, and is particularly valuable for applied contexts due to the richness of the produced narrative. Using the methodological means supplied by the fieldwork-oriented research in drug use studies would offer new insights for scholars and policy makers. We present the methodological argument regarding the strategic use of the principles of ethnographic approach to increase the informativeness, accuracy and validity of the results in applied research on recreational drug use. Besides the methodological innovations the fieldwork-oriented research offers, using the methodological means supplied by the ethnographic research on cultural models would enable the social researchers to address the problem of drug use more efficiently.
Keywords: ethnographic methods, cultural models theory, recreational drug use, research design
Publication in: eng
References
Ahmed, S. (2011). Toward an evolutionary basis for resilience to drug addiction. Behavioral аnd Brain Sciences, 34 (6), 310-311. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x11000677
Banaschewski, T., Blomeyer, D., Buchmann, A., Poustka, L., Rothenberger, A., Laucht, M. (2011). Drugs as instruments from a developmental child and adolescent psychiatric perspective. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34 (6), 312-313. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x11000690
Becker, H. (1966). Outsiders: Studies in the Sociology of Deviance. London: Free Press.
Becker, H. (1967). History, Culture and Subjective Experience: An Exploration of the Social Bases of Drug-Induced Experiences. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 8 (3), 163. https://doi.org/10.2307/2948371
Bennardo, G., de Munck, V. (2013). Cultural models: Genesis, methods, and experiences. S.l.: Oxford University Press.
Blount, B. (2011). A history of cognitive anthropology. In: D.B. Kronenfeld, G. Bennardo, V.C. de Munck, M. Fischer (Eds.), A Companion to Cognitive Anthropology (pp. 11–29). Oxford: Blackwell. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781444394931
Bourgois, P. (2018). Decolonising drug studies in an era of predatory accumulation. Third World Quarterly, 39 (2), 385-398. https://doi.org/10.1080/01436597.2017.1411187
Caulkin, D.D. (2004). Identifying culture as a threshold of shared knowledge: A Consensus analysis method. International Journal of Cross Cultural Management, 4 (3), 317–333. https://doi.org/10.1177/1470595804047813
Chiu et al. (2010). Intersubjective culture: The role of intersubjective perceptions in cross-cultural research. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5 (4), 482–493. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691610375562
Commonly Abused Drugs (2020). Retrieved from: https://www.drugabuse.gov/sites/default/files/cadc...
D’Andrade, R. (1995). The Development of Cognitive Anthropology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
D’Andrade, R. (2002). Violence without honor in the American South. In: T. Aase (Ed.), Tournaments of Power: Honor and Revenge in the Contemporary World (pp. 61–75). Burlington: Ashgate. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315235950
D’Andrade, R. (2008). Study of Personal and Cultural Values: American, Japanese and Vietnamese. New York, NY: Palgrave Macmillan.
D’Andrade, R., Strauss, C. (1992). Human Motives and Cultural Models. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Dasgupta, N., Beletsky, L., Ciccarone, D. (2018). Opioid crisis: No easy fix to its social and economic determinants. American Journal of Public Health, 108 (2), 182–186. https://doi.org/10.2105/ajph.2017.304187
de Munck, V., Bennardo, G. (2019). Disciplining сulture: A sociocognitive approach. Current Anthropology, 60 (2), 174–193. https://doi.org/10.1086/702470
DiMaggio, P. (1997). Culture and cognition. Annual Review of Sociology, 23, (1), 263–87.
European Drug Report (2019). Retrieved from: http://www.emcdda.europa.eu/system/files/publicati...
European Monitoring Centre for Drugs and Drug Addiction (EMCDDA) (2017). Health and Social Responses to Drug Problems: A European Guide. S.l.
Foxall, G., Sigurdsson, V. (2011). Drug use as consumer behavior. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34(6), 313–314. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x11000707
Friedman, S., Mateu-Gelabert, P., Sandoval, M., Hagan, H., Jarlais, D. (2008). Positive deviance control-case life history: a method to develop grounded hypotheses about successful long-term avoidance of infection. BMC Public Health, 8(1). https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-8-94
Gilbert, M. (1987). Modeling collective belief. Synthese, 73, 185–204
Gilbert, M. (2015). Joint commitment. How we make a social world. New York: Oxford University Press.
Global Drug Survey (2016). Key findings from the Global Drug Survey 2016. https://doi.org/10.18356/fa014eb4-en
Goodenough, W. (1971). Culture, Language and Society. S.l.: Addison-Wesley.
Goudie, A., Gullo, M., Rose, A., Christiansen, P., Cole, J., Field, M., Sumnall, H. (2011). Nonaddictive instrumental drug use: Theoretical strengths and weaknesses. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34 (6), 314–315. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x11000719
Griffiths, M. (2011). Non-addictive psychoactive drug use: Implications for behavioral addiction. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34 (6), 315–316. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x11000720
Lareau, A. (2015). Cultural knowledge and social inequality. American Sociological Review, 80(1), 1–27.
Lee, C., Neighbors, C., Woods, B. (2007). Marijuana motives: Youngadults' reasons for using marijuana. Addictive Behaviors, 32(7), 1384–1394. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2006.09.010
Maltseva, K. (2016). Using correspondence analysis of scales as part of mixed methods design to access cultural models in ethnographic fieldwork: Prosocial cooperation in Sweden. Journal of Mixed Methods Research, 10 (1), 82–111. https://doi.org/10.1177/1558689814525262
Maltseva, K. (forthcoming). Bridging sociology with anthropology and cognitive science perspectives to assess shared cultural knowledge. _Sociology: Theory, Methods, Marketing._https://doi.org/10.15407/sociology2020.01.108
Maltseva, K., D’Andrade, R. (2011). Multi-item scales and cognitive ethnography. In: D.B. Kronenfeld, G. Bennardo, V.C. de Munck, M. Fischer (Eds.), A Companion to Cognitive Anthropology (pp. 153–170). Oxford: Blackwell. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781444394931.ch9
Miller, G. (2011). Optimal drug use and rational drug policy. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34 (6), 318–319. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x11000756
Moore, K., Miles, S. (2004). Young people, dance and the sub-cultural consumption of drugs. Addiction Research & Theory, 12 (6), 507–523. https://doi.org/10.1080/16066350412331323083
Müller, C., Schumann, G. (2011). Drugs as instruments: A new framework for non-addictive psychoactive drug use. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34 (6), 293–310. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x11000057
Nicholson, T., Duncan, D., White, J. (2002). Is recreational drug use normal? Journal of Substance Use, 7(3), 116–123. https://doi.org/10.1080/14659890209169340
Nutt, D., King, L., Saulsbury, W., Blakemore, C. (2007). Development of a rational scale to assess the harm of drugs of potential misuse. The Lancet, 369 (9566), 1047–1053. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0140-6736(07)60464-4
Pickard, H. (2011). The instrumental rationality of addiction. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34(6), 320-321. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x1100077x
Polavieja, J.G. (2015). Capturing culture: A new method to estimate exogenous cultural effects using migrant populations. American Sociological Review, 80(1), 166–191. https://doi.org/10.1177/0003122414562600
Prayag, G., Mura, P., Hall, C., Fontaine, J. (2016). Spirituality, drugs, and tourism: tourists’ and shamans’ experiences of ayahuasca in Iquitos, Peru. Tourism Recreation Research, 41(3), 314–325. https://doi.org/10.1080/02508281.2016.1192237
Quinn, N. (1996). Culture and contradiction: The case of Americans reasoning about marriage. Ethos, 24, 391–425. https://doi.org/10.1525/eth.1996.24.3.02a00010
Quinn, N. (2005). Finding Culture in Talk: A Collection of Methods. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.
Quinn, N. (2011). The history of the cultural models school reconsidered. In: D. B. Kronenfeld, G.Bennardo, V. C. de Munck, M. Fischer (Eds.), A Companion to Cognitive Anthropology (pp. 30–46). Oxford: Blackwell. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781444394931
Quinn, N. (2018). An anthropologist’s view of American marriage: Limitations of the tool kit theory of culture. In: N. Quinn (Ed.), Advances in Culture Theory from Psychological Anthropology (pp. 139–184). New York: Palgrave Macmillan. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-93674-1_6
Quinn, N., Holland, D. (1987). Culture and cognition. In: D. Holland, N. Quinn (Eds.), Cultural Models in Language and Thought (pp. 3–42). London: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/cbo9780511607660.002
Ravn, S., Duff, C. (2015). Putting the party down on paper: A novel method for mapping youth drug use in private settings. Health & Place, 31, 124–132. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.healthplace.2014.11.010
Ross, N. (2004). Culture and cognition: Implications for theory and method. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Seligmann, L., Estes, B. (2019). Innovations in ethnographic methods. American Behavioral Scientist, 64 (2), 176–197. https://doi.org/10.1177/0002764219859640
Shimizu, H. (2011). Cognitive approaches and education: foundational models of self and cultural models of teaching and learning in Japan and the United States. In: D.B. Kronenfeld, G. Bennardo, V.C. de Munck, M. Fischer (Eds.), A Companion to Cognitive Anthropology (pp. 430–449). Oxford: Blackwell. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781444394931.ch23
Romney, A.K., Weller, S. C., Batchelder, W.H. (1986). Culture as consensus: A theory of cultural and informant accuracy. American Anthropologist, 88, 313–338. https://doi.org/10.1525/aa.1986.88.2.02a00020
Singer, M., Page, J. (2016). The Social Value of Drug Addicts. London: Routledge.
Soussan, C., Andersson, M., Kjellgren, A. (2018). The diverse reasons for using Novel Psychoactive Substances - A qualitative study of the users' own perspectives. International Journal of Drug Policy, 52, 71–78. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugpo.2017.11.003
Strauss, C. (2000). The culture concept and the individualism/collectivism debate: Dominant and alternative attributions for class in the United States. In: L. Nucci, G. Saxe, E. Turiel (Eds.), Culture, Thought, and Development (pp. 85–114). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Suizzo, A.M. (2002). French parents’ cultural models and childrearing beliefs. _International Journal of Behavioral Development,_26, 297–307. https://doi.org/10.1080/01650250143000175
Swendsen, J., Le Moal, M. (2011). Flaws of drug instrumentalization. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34 (6), 323–324. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x11000732
Taylor, S.P., Hulsizer, M.R. (1998). Psychoactive drugs and human aggression. In: R.G. Geen, E. Donnerstein (Eds.), Human aggression: Theories, research, and implications for social policy (pp. 139–165). New York: Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/b978-012278805-5/50007-9
United Nations Office on Drugs and Crime (UNODC) (2018). World Drug Report 2018. United Nations publication, E.18.XI.9_._https://doi.org/10.18356/d29e3f27-en
Vaisey, S. (2009). Motivation and justification: A dual-process model of culture in action. American Journal of Sociology, 114, 1675–1715. https://doi.org/10.1086/597179
Van Gulick, R. (2011). Drugs, mental instruments, and self-control. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34 (6), 325–326. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x1100080x
Volkow, N., Collins, F. (2017). The role of science in addressing the opioid crisis. New England Journal of Medicine, 377 (4), 391–394. https://doi.org/10.1056/nejmsr1706626
Weller, S.C. (2007). Frequently asked questions about consensus analysis. _Field Methods,_19, 339–368. https://doi.org/10.1177/1525822x07303502
Weller, S.C., Romney, A.K. (1990). Metric Scaling. Correspondence Analysis. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Wu, K. (2011). Governing drug use through neurobiological subject construction: The sad loss of the sociocultural. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34 (6), 327–328. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x11000835
Zerubavel, E. (1999). Social Mindscapes. An Invitation to Cognitive Sociology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Received 12.04.2020